The Korean Association for the Study of English Language and Linguistics
[ Article ]
Korea Journal of English Language and Linguistics - Vol. 22, No. 0, pp.70-85
ISSN: 1598-1398 (Print) 2586-7474 (Online)
Print publication date 31 Jan 2022
Received 18 Dec 2021 Revised 16 Feb 2022 Accepted 27 Feb 2022
DOI: https://doi.org/10.15738/kjell.22..202202.70

Predictive Sentence Processing: Evidence from Passive Relative Clause Processing

Nayoun Kim ; Jiayi Lu
(1st / corresponding author) Assistant Professor, Department of English Language and Literature, Sungkyunkwan Univ. nayoun@skku.edu
Ph.D student, Department of Linguistics, Stanford Univ. jiayi.lu@stanford.edu


© 2022 KASELL All rights reserved
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons License, which permits unrestricted non-commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Abstract

Previous studies proposed that readers engage in predictive processing by showing that the processing of expected elements is facilitated compared to unexpected elements (Frazier and d’Arcais 1989, Frazier and Clifton 1989, Stowe 1986). However, others claim that such processing facilitation can also be attributed to integration facilitation (Gibson 1998, 2000, Grodner and Gibson 2005). Using a self-paced reading experiment, this study provides evidence for predictive processing by examining the processing of passive relative clauses with pied-piped by-phrases. We found evidence that the processing of inanimate embedded subjects is facilitated after encountering by-phrases, an effect that can only be attributed to predictive processing rather than integration facilitation. Overall, this study provides further evidence that readers engage in a predictive structure-building processes ahead of encountering unequivocal bottom-up input (Phillips 2006, Staub and Clifton 2006, Wagers and Phillips 2009, Yoshida 2006, Yoshida, Dickey and Sturt 2013).

Keywords:

structural prediction, passive relative clause processing, pied-piping, p-stranding, animacy, integration, moving-window self-paced reading experiment

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank anonymous reviewers for their valuable comments. We would also like to thank Masaya Yoshida for his helpful discussions.

References

  • Abney, S. P. and M. Johnson. 1991. Memory requirements and local ambiguities of parsing strategies. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 20(3), 233-250. [https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01067217]
  • Altmann, G. and Y. Kamide. 1999. Incremental interpretation at verbs: Restricting the domain of subsequent reference. Cognition 73, 247-264. [https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00059-1]
  • Arnett, N. and M. Wagers. 2017. Subject encodings and retrieval interference. Journal of Memory and Language 93, 22-54. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2016.07.005]
  • Baayen, R. H. 2008. Analyzing Linguistic Data: A Practical Introduction to Statistics Using R. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511801686]
  • Baayen, R. H., D. J. Davidson and D. M. Bates. 2008. Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language 59(4), 390-412. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.005]
  • Baker, M., K. Johnson and I. Roberts. 1989. Passive arguments raised. Linguistic Inquiry 20(2), 219-251.
  • Barr, D. J., R. Levy, C. Scheepers and H. J. Tily. 2013. Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language 68(3), 255-278. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001]
  • Boland, J. E. 2005. Visual arguments. Cognition 95(3), 237-274. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2004.01.008]
  • Box, G. E. P. and D. R. Cox. 1964. An analysis of transformations. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series B (Methodological) 26(2), 211-252. [https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2517-6161.1964.tb00553.x]
  • Chen, E., E. Gibson and F. Wolf. 2005. Online syntactic storage costs in sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language 52, 144-169. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2004.10.001]
  • Chomsky, N. 1977. On wh-movement. In P. W. Culicover, T. Wasow and A. Akmajian, eds., Formal Syntax, 71-132. New York: Academic Press.
  • Chomsky, N. 1981. Lectures on Government and Binding. Dordrecht: Foris.
  • Chung, S., W. A. Ladusaw and J. McCloskey. 1995. Sluicing and logical form. Natural Language Semantics 3, 239-282. [https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01248819]
  • Crocker, Matthew W. 1996. Computational Psycholinguistics: An Interdisciplinary Approach to the Study of Language: Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics; V. 20. Dordrecht; Boston: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
  • DeLong, K. A., Urbach, T. P. and M. Kutas. 2005. Probabilistic word pre-activation during language comprehension inferred from electrical brain activity. Nature Neuroscience 8(8), 1117-1121. [https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1504]
  • Dowty, D. 1991. Thematic proto-roles and argument selection. Language 67, 547-619. [https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.1991.0021]
  • Drummond, A. 2013. Ibex farm [Computer software]. http://spellout.net/ibexfarm
  • Federmeier, K. D. 2007. Thinking ahead: The role and roots of prediction in language comprehension. Psychophysiology 44, 491-505. [https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00531.x]
  • Federmeier, K. D. and M. Kutas. 1999. A rose by any other name: Long-term memory structure and sentence processing. Journal of Memory and Language 41, 469-495 [https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1999.2660]
  • Ferreira, F. 1994. Choice of passive voice is affected by verb type and animacy. Journal of Memory and Language 33, 715-736. [https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1994.1034]
  • Ferreira, F. 2003. The misinterpretation of noncanonical sentences. Cognitive Psychology 47, 164-203. [https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0285(03)00005-7]
  • Ferreira, F. and B. Swets. 2002. How incremental is language production? Evidence from the production of utterances requiring the computation of arithmetic sums. Journal of Memory and Language 46(1), 57-84. [https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2001.2797]
  • Frazier, L. and Clifton, Jr. 1989. Successive cyclicity in the grammar and the parser. Language and Cognitive Processes 4(2), 93-126. [https://doi.org/10.1080/01690968908406359]
  • Frazier, L. and J. D. Fodor. 1978. The sausage machine: A new two-stage parsing model. Cognition 6, 291-325. [https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(78)90002-1]
  • Frazier, L. and G. B. Flores d’Arcais. 1989. Filler driven parsing: A study of gap filling in Dutch. Journal of Memory and Language 28(3), 331-344. [https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(89)90037-5]
  • Fredriksson, L. 2016. A Corpus-based Contrastive Study of the Passive and Related Constructions in English and Swedish. Doctoral dissertation, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden.
  • Gibson, E. 1998. Syntactic complexity: locality of syntactic dependencies. Cognition 68(1), 1-76. [https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00034-1]
  • Gibson, E. 2000. The dependency locality theory: A distance-based theory of linguistic complexity. In A. Marantz, Y. Miyashita and W. O’Neil, eds., Image, Language, Brain: Papers from the First Mind Articulation Project Symposium, 94-126. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Gibson, E. and T. Warren. 2004. Reading time evidence for intermediate linguistic structure in long-distance dependencies. Syntax 7(1), 55-78. [https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1368-0005.2004.00065.x]
  • Grimshaw, J. 1990. Argument Structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Grodner, D. and E. Gibson. 2005. Consequences of the serial nature of linguistic input for sentential complexity. Cognitive Science 29(2), 261-291. [https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog0000_7]
  • Jackendoff, R. and B. Landau. 1991. Spatial language and spatial cognition. In D. J. Napoli and J. A. Kegl, eds., Bridges Between Psychology and Linguistics: A Swarthmore Festschrift for Lila Gleitman, 144-169. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Jaeger, T. F. 2008. Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language 59(4), 434-446. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.11.007]
  • Jaeggli, O. 1986. Passive. Linguistic Inquiry 17, 587-622.
  • Just, M. A., P. A. Carpenter and J. D. Woolley. 1982. Paradigms and processes in reading comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology 111(2): 228-238. [https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.111.2.228]
  • Kamide, Y., G. Altmann and S. Haywood. 2003. The time-course of prediction in incremental sentence processing: Evidence from anticipatory eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language 49(1), 133-156. [https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00023-8]
  • Keine, S. 2020. Locality domains in syntax: Evidence from sentence processing. Syntax 23, 105-151. [https://doi.org/10.1111/synt.12195]
  • Kazanina, N. 2017. Predicting complex syntactic structure in real time: Processing of negative sentences in Russian. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 70(11), 2200-2218. [https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.1228684]
  • Kim, N., L. Brehm, P. Sturt and M. Yoshida. 2020. How long can you hold the filler: maintenance and retrieval. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 35(1), 17-42. [https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2019.1626456]
  • Kim, N., K. Carlson, M. Dickey and M. Yoshida. 2020. Processing gapping: Parallelism and grammatical constraints. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 73(5), 781-798. [https://doi.org/10.1177/1747021820903461]
  • Kimball, J. P. 1973. Seven principles of surface structure parsing in natural languages. Cognition 2(1), 15-47. [https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(72)90028-5]
  • Kutas, M. and K. D. Federmeier. 2011. Thirty years and counting: finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology 62, 621-647. [https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.131123]
  • Larson, R. K. 1985. On the syntax of disjunction scope. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 3, 217-264. [https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00133841]
  • Lasnik, H. 1988. Subjects and the theta-criterion. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 6, 1-17. [https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01791588]
  • Lasnik, H. 2001. When can you save a structure by destroying it? Proceedings of the 31st North East Linguistics Society, 301-320.
  • Lasnik, H. 2005. Review of Jason Merchant: The syntax of silence. Language 81, 259-265. [https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2005.0025]
  • Lau, E., C. Stroud, S. Plesch and C. Phillips. 2006. The role of structural prediction in rapid syntactic analysis. Brain and Language 98, 74-88. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2006.02.003]
  • Lee, M. W. 2004. Another look at the role of empty categories in sentence processing (and grammar). Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 33(1), 51-73. [https://doi.org/10.1023/B:JOPR.0000010514.50468.30]
  • Mahowald, K., P. Graff, J. Hartman and E. Gibson. 2016. SNAP Judgments: A Small N Acceptability Paradigm (SNAP) for Linguistic Acceptability Judgments. Language: Journal of the Linguistic Society of America 92, 619-635. [https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2016.0052]
  • Marantz, A. 1984. On the Nature of Grammatical Relations. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Merchant, J. 2001. The Syntax of Silence: Sluicing, Islands, and the Theory of Ellipsis. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Phillips, C. 2006. The real-time status of island phenomena. Language 82(4), 795-823. [https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2006.0217]
  • Sloggett, S. and M. Yoshida. 2019. Presentation at Chicago Area Sentence Processing Experiments and Research (CASPER). Chicago, IL.
  • Staub, A. 2010. Eye movements and processing difficulty in object relative clauses. Cognition 116(1), 71-86. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.04.002]
  • Staub, A. and C. Clifton, Jr. 2006. Syntactic prediction in language comprehension: Evidence from either ... or. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 32(2), 425-436. [https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.32.2.425]
  • Stowe, L. A. 1986. Parsing wh-constructions: Evidence for online gap location. Language and Cognitive Processes 1(3), 227-245. [https://doi.org/10.1080/01690968608407062]
  • Sturt, P. 2003. The time-course of the application of binding constraints in reference resolution. Journal of Memory and Language 48(3), 542-562. [https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00536-3]
  • Sturt, P. and V. Lombardo. 2005. Processing coordinated structures: Incrementality and connectedness. Cognitive Science 29(2), 291-305. [https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog0000_8]
  • Sprouse, J. and D. Almeida. 2017. Design sensitivity and statistical power in acceptability judgment experiments. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics 2, 1-32. [https://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.236]
  • Van Berkum, J. J., C. M. Brown, P. Zwitserlood, V. Kooijman and P. Hagoort. 2005. Anticipating upcoming words in discourse: Evidence from ERPs and reading times. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 31(3), 443. [https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.31.3.443]
  • Van Gompel, R. P. and S. P. Liversedge. 2003. The influence of morphological information on cataphoric pronoun assignment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 29(1), 128-139. [https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.29.1.128]
  • Vasishth, S. and R. L. Lewis. 2006. Argument-head distance and processing complexity: Explaining both locality and antilocality effects. Language 82(4), 767-794. [https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2006.0236]
  • Wagers, M. and C. Phillips. 2009. Multiple dependencies and the role of the grammar in real-time comprehension. Journal of Linguistics 45, 395-433. [https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022226709005726]
  • Warren, T. and E. Gibson. 2002. The influence of referential processing on sentence complexity. Cognition 85(1), 79-112. [https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00087-2]
  • Yoshida, M. 2006. Constraints and Mechanisms in Long-distance Dependency Formation. University of Maryland, College Park.
  • Yoshida, M., M. W. Dickey and P. Sturt. 2013. Predictive processing of syntactic structure: Sluicing and ellipsis in real-time sentence processing. Language and Cognitive Processes 28(3), 272-302. [https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2011.622905]