The Korean Association for the Study of English Language and Linguistics
[ Article ]
Korea Journal of English Language and Linguistics - Vol. 23, No. 0, pp.913-934
ISSN: 1598-1398 (Print) 2586-7474 (Online)
Print publication date 30 Jan 2023
Received 10 Jul 2023 Revised 31 Aug 2023 Accepted 15 Oct 2023
DOI: https://doi.org/10.15738/kjell.23..202310.913

The Effect of Referential Forms on Incremental Processing of Long-distance Dependencies: Evidence from English Cleft Sentences

Myung Hye Yoo
Postdoctoral Researcher, Dept. of English, Linguistics and Theatre Studies. National University of Singapore mhyoo@nus.edu.sg


© 2023 KASELL All rights reserved
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons License, which permits unrestricted non-commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Abstract

This study examines how the referential form of the noun phrase, intervening the dependency between the filler and gap, modulates the long-distance dependency formation. (e.g., It was the actor who the director graciously thanked before the show). Two theories are evaluated in two self-paced reading tasks: expectation-based and memory-based accounts. The memory-based theories predict later difficulty at the verb (e.g., thanked), whereas the expectation-based process predicts earlier difficulty at the intervenor (e.g., the director). In terms of memory-based theories, this paper explores the reading time at the verb region to determine whether the same referential form of the intervenor with the filler exhibits processing load (similarity-based interference) or whether the parser is sensitive to the gradient status of the intervenor in a discourse within the framework of the dependency locality theory (Givenness Hierarchy). Experiment 1 showed that the verb was read faster with a pronoun intervenor compared to a definite or indefinite intervenor, indicating the binary nature of the Givenness Hierarchy. No interaction between the filler and the intervenor was revealed, indicating no similarity-based interference effects. Experiment 2 confirmed that the insensitivity to different types of full NPs at the verb region cannot be due to contextual support and did not observe similarity effects. Expectation-based processing was also evident at the intervenor region in both Experiments 1 and 2. In Experiment 1, immediate processing difficulty emerged upon encountering an unexpected referential form. In Experiment 2, processing ease for the intervenor was observed when the previously parsed filler NP led to a strong prediction of the NP type of an intervenor. This study contributes to the growing body of evidence that both memory- and expectation-based processes contribute to the incremental processing of complex sentences involving long-distance dependencies, highlighting distinct processing patterns at different regions.

Keywords:

filler-gap dependencies, referential processing, memory-based parsing, expectation-based parsing, cleft sentences

Acknowledgments

This paper was developed from part of a Ph.D. dissertation by the author.

References

  • Abeillé, A., B. Hemforth, E. Winckel and E. Gibson. 2020. Extraction from subjects: Differences in acceptability depend on the discourse function of the construction. Cognition 204. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104293]
  • Arnold, J. 2001. The effect of thematic roles on pronoun use and frequency of reference continuation. Discourse Processes 31(2), 137-162. [https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326950DP3102_02]
  • Arnold, J. 2010. How speakers refer: The role of accessibility. Language and Linguistics Compass, 187-203. [https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-818X.2010.00193.x]
  • Arnold, J., J. Eisenband, S. Brown-Schmidt and J. Trueswell. 2000. The rapid use of gender information: Evidence of the time course of pronoun resolution from eye tracking. Cognition 76(1), B13-B26. [https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00073-1]
  • Baayen, R. H., D. J. Davidson and D. M. Bates 2008. Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language 59, 390-412. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.005]
  • Bade, N. and F. Schwarz. 2019. An experimental investigation of antipresuppositions. In A. Creemers and C. Richter, eds., University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics, 25(1), 5. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania.
  • Bade, N. and F. Schwarz. 2021. New Data on the Competition Between Definites and Indefinites. In Proceedings of Experiments in Linguistic Meeting, Volume 1, 15-26, Washington, DC: Linguistic Society of America. [https://doi.org/10.3765/elm.1.4894]
  • Barr, D. J., R. Levy, C. Scheepers and H. J. Tily. 2013. Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language 68(3), 255-278. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001]
  • Bates, D. 2010. Lme4: Mixed-Effects Modeling with R.
  • Bates, D., M. Maechler, B. Bolker and S. Walker. 2015. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. ArXiv Preprint. [https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01]
  • Belletti, A. 2015. The focus map of clefts: Extraposition and predication. In U. Shlonsky, ed., Beyond functional sequence vol. 10, 42-60. [https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780190210588.003.0003]
  • Boston, M. F., J. Hale, R. Kliegl, U. Patil and S. Vasishth. 2008. Parsing costs as predictors of reading difficulty: An evaluation using the Potsdam sentence corpus. Journal of Eye Movement Research 2, 1-12. [https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.2.1.1]
  • Chomsky, N. 1977. On wh-movement. In P., Culicover, T., Wasow and A. Akmajian, eds., Formal Syntax 71-132. Academic Press.
  • Corley, S. and M. W. Crocker. 2000. The modular statistical hypothesis: Exploring lexical category ambiguity. In M. W. Crocker, M. Pickering and C. J. Clifton, eds., Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing 135-160. Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press. [https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511527210.007]
  • Cottell, S. 2002. The Comparative Syntax of Cleft Constructions. Doctoral dissertation, University of Wales, Cardiff, UK.
  • Culicover, P. and S. Winkler. 2022. Parasitic gaps aren’t parasitic or the case of the Uninvited Guest. The Linguistic Review 39(1), 1-35. [https://doi.org/10.1515/tlr-2021-2080]
  • Delahunty, G. 1981. Topics in the Syntax and Semantics of English Cleft Sentences. Doctoral dissertation, University of California, Irvine, Irvine, CA, USA.
  • Demberg, V. and F. Keller. 2008. Data from eye-tracking corpora as evidence for theories of syntactic processing complexity. Cognition 109(2), 193-210. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.07.008]
  • Drummond, A. 2018. Ibex Farm. http://spellout.net/ibexfarm, .
  • Erteschik-Shir, N. 1973. On the Nature of Island B Constraints. Doctoral dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA.
  • Fedorenko, E., E. Gibson and D. Rohde. 2006. The nature of working memory capacity in sentence comprehension: Evidence against domain-specific working memory resources. Journal of Memory and Language 54, 541-553. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2005.12.006]
  • Fedorenko, E., R. Woodbury, R and E. Gibson. 2009. Making the Object Noun Phrase More Easily Retrievable from Memory Facilitates the Processing of Object-Extracted Relative Clauses: Direct Evidence for Memory-Based Accounts. In The 22nd CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing, Davis, CA. University of California.
  • Ferreira, F. and C. Clifton. 1986. The independence of syntactic processing. Journal of Memory and Language 25, 348-368. [https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(86)90006-9]
  • Fletcher, C. 1984. Markedness and topic continuity in discourse processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 23(4), 487-493. [https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(84)90309-8]
  • Fukumura, K. and R. P. G. van Gompel. 2010. Choosing anaphoric expression: Do people take into account likelihood of reference? Journal of Memory and Language 62(1), 52-66. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2009.09.001]
  • Gazdar, G., E. Klein, G. K. Pullum, G and I. Sag. 1985. Generalized Phrase Structure Grammar. Blackwell.
  • Gibson, E. 1998. Linguistic complexity: Locality of syntactic dependencies. Cognition 68, 1-76. [https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00034-1]
  • Gibson, E. 2000. The dependency locality theory: a distance-based theory of linguistic complexity. In Y. Miyashita, A. P. Marantz and W. O’Neil, eds., Image, Language, Brain 95-126. Cambridge: MIT Press.
  • Gibson, E. and I. Wu. 2011. Processing Chinese relative clauses in context. Language and Cognitive Processes 1-31.
  • Givon, T. 1984. Syntax: A Functional–typological Introduction, vol. 1. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing. [https://doi.org/10.1075/z.17]
  • Goldberg, A. 2006. Constructions at Work. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Gordon, P. C., R. Hendrick and M. Johnson. 2001. Memory interference during language processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology 27(6), 1411-1423. [https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.27.6.1411]
  • Gordon, P. C., R. Hendrick and M. Johnson. 2004. Effects of noun phrase type on sentence complexity. Journal of Memory and Language 51, 97-114. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2004.02.003]
  • Gordon, P. C., R. Hendrick, M. Johnson and Y. Lee. 2006. Similarity-Based Interference During Language Comprehension: Evidence from Eye Tracking During Reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology 32(6), 1304–1321. [https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.32.6.1304]
  • Gordon, P. C., R. Hendrick and W. H. Levine. 2002. Memory-load interference in syntactic processing. Psychological Science 13, 425-430. [https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00475]
  • Grodner, D., E. Gibson and D. Watson. 2005. The influence of contextual contrast on syntactic processing: Evidence for strong-interaction in sentence comprehension. Cognition 95, 275-296. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2004.01.007]
  • Gundel, J. K. 2010. Reference and accessibility from a Givenness Hierarchy perspective. International Review of Pragmatics 2, 148-168. [https://doi.org/10.1163/187731010X528322]
  • Gundel, J. K., N. Hedberg and R. Zacharski. 1993. Cognitive status and the form of referring expressions in discourse. Language 69(2), 274-307. [https://doi.org/10.2307/416535]
  • Gundel, J. K., N. Hedberg and R. Zacharski. 2012. Underspecification of cognitive status in reference production: Some empirical predictions. Topics in Cognitive Science 4, 249-268. [https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2012.01184.x]
  • Hale, J. 2001. A probabilistic Earley parser as a psycholinguistic model. North American Chapter of the Association for Computational Linguistics (NAACL) 159-166. [https://doi.org/10.3115/1073336.1073357]
  • Hedberg, N. 1990. The Discourse Function of Cleft Sentences in English. Doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA.
  • Hedberg, N. 2000. On the referential status of clefts. Language 76, 891-920. [https://doi.org/10.2307/417203]
  • Heggie, L. 1988. The Syntax of Copular Structures. Doctoral dissertation, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, USA.
  • Heim, I. 1982. The Semantics of Definite and Indefinite NPs. Doctoral dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, MA, USA.
  • Heim, I. 1983. File change semantics and the familiarity theory of definiteness. In Semantics Critical Concepts in Linguistics 108-135. [https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110852820.164]
  • Hofmeister, P. 2011. Representational complexity and memory retrieval in language comprehension. Language Cognitive Processes 26(3), 109-123. [https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2010.492642]
  • Hofmeister, P. and E. Norcliffe. 2014. Does resumption facilitate sentence comprehension? In P. Hofmeister and E. Norcliffe, eds., The Core and the Periphery: Data-Driven Perspectives on Syntax Inspired by Ivan A. Sag, 225-246. CA: Centre for the Study of Language and Information.
  • Hofmeister, P. and S. Vasishth. 2014. Distinctiveness and encoding effects in online sentence comprehension. Frontiers in Psychology 5, 1237. [https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01237]
  • Jacoby, L. L. and L. R. Brooks. 1984. Nonanalytic cognition: Memory, perception and concept learning. In G. Bower, ed., Psychology of Learning and Motivation vol. 18, 1-47. Cambridge: Academic Press. [https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60358-8]
  • Jäger, L., F. Engelmann and S. Vasishth. 2017. Similarity-based interference in sentence comprehension: Literature review and Bayesian meta-analysis. Journal of Memory and Language 94, 316-339. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2017.01.004]
  • Jurafsky, D. 2003. Probabilistic modeling in psycholinguistics: Linguistic comprehension and production. In R. Bod, J. Hay and S. Jannedy, eds., Probabilistic Linguistics 39-95. MIT Press. [https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/5582.003.0006]
  • Just, M. A., P. A. Carpenter and J. D. Woolley. 1982. Paradigms and processes and in reading comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 3(2), 228-238. [https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.111.2.228]
  • Kaiser, E. and J. C. Trueswell. 2008. Interpreting pronouns and demonstratives in Finnish: Evidence for a form-specific approach to reference resolution. Language and Cognitive Processes 23(5), 709-748. [https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960701771220]
  • Kirsten, M., S. Tiemann, V. Seibold, I. Hertrichm, S. Beck and B. Rolke. 2014. When the polar bear encounters many polar bears: event-related potential context effects evoked by uniqueness failure. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 29(9), 1147-1162. [https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2014.899378]
  • Laurinavichyute, A. 2020. Similarity-based Interference and Faulty Encoding Accounts of Sentence Processing. Doctoral dissertation, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany.
  • Leonetti, M. 2004. Specificity and differential object marking in Spanish. Catalan Journal of Linguistics 3, 75-114. [https://doi.org/10.5565/rev/catjl.106]
  • Levy, R. 2008. Expectation-based syntactic comprehension. Cognition 106(3), 599-604. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.05.006]
  • Lewis, R. L. and S. Vasishth. 2005. An activation-based model of sentence processing as skilled memory retrieval. Cognitive Science 29(3), 375-419. [https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog0000_25]
  • Lewis, R. L., S. Vasishth and J. A. Van Dyke. 2006. Computational principles of working memory in sentence comprehension. TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences 10(10), 447-454. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.08.007]
  • Löbner, S. 1985. Definites. Journal of Semantics 4, 279-326. [https://doi.org/10.1093/jos/4.4.279]
  • MacDonald, M. C., N. J. Pearlmutter and M. S. Seidenberg. 1994a. Lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution. Psychological Review 101(4), 676-703. [https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.101.4.676]
  • MacDonald, M. C., N. J. Pearlmutter and M. S. Seidenberg. 1994b. Syntactic ambiguity resolution as lexical ambiguity resolution. In C. Clifton, L. Frazier and K. Rayner, eds., Perspectives on Sentence Processing 123-154. New York: Psychology Press.
  • MacDonald, M. and M. Christiansen. 2002. Reassessing working memory: Comment on Just and Carpenter (1992) and Waters and Caplan (1996). Psychological Review 109(1), 35-54. [https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.109.1.35]
  • Masharov, M. 2008. Reference Resolution and Discourse Salience. Doctoral dissertation, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA.
  • Price, I. and J. Witzel. 2017. Sources of relative clause processing difficulty: Evidence from Russian. Journal of Memory and Language 208-244. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2017.07.013]
  • Reali, F. and M. H. Christiansen. 2007. Processing of relative clauses is made easier by frequency of occurrence. Journal of Memory and Language 57, 1-23. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2006.08.014]
  • Reeve, M. 2011. The syntactic structure of English clefts. Lingua 121, 142-171. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2010.05.004]
  • Roark, B., A. Bachrach, C. Cardenas and C. Pallier. 2009. Deriving Lexical and Syntactic Expectation-based Measures for Psycholinguistic Modeling via Incremental Top-down Parsing. In Proceedings of the 2009 Conference on Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing, 324-333, Singapore, Association for Computational Linguistics. [https://doi.org/10.3115/1699510.1699553]
  • Rochemont, M. 1986. Focus in Generative Grammar. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. [https://doi.org/10.1075/sigla.4]
  • Roland, D., F. Dick and J. Elman. 2007. Frequency of basic English grammatical structures: A corpus analysis. Journal of Memory and Language 57, 348-379. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.03.002]
  • Roland, D. and D. Jurafsky. 2002. Verb sense and verb subcategorization probabilities. In S. Stevenson and P. Merlo, eds., The Lexical Basis of Sentence Processing: Formal, Computational and Experimental Issues 325-346. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. [https://doi.org/10.1075/nlp.4.17rol]
  • Smith, N. and R. Levy. 2013. The effect of word predictability on reading time is logarithmic. Cognition 128(3), 302-319. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.02.013]
  • Spivey-Knowlton, M. and J. Sedivy. 1995. Resolving attachment ambiguities with multiple constraints. Cognition 55, 227-267. [https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(94)00647-4]
  • Staub, A. 2010. Response time distributional evidence for distinct varieties of number attraction. Cognition 114, 447-454. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.11.003]
  • Takami, K. 1992. Preposition stranding: from syntactic to functional analysis. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. [https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110870398]
  • Tiemann, S., M. Schmid, N. Bade, B. Rolke, I. Hertrich, H. Ackermann, J. Knapp and S. Beck. 2011. Psycholinguistic Evidence for Presuppositions: On-line and Off-line Data. In Proceedings of Sinn & Bedeutung, Volume 15, 581-595, Saarbrücken: Saarland University Press.
  • Trueswell, J. C. 1996. The role of lexical frequency in syntactic ambiguity resolution. Journal of Memory and Language 35, 566-585. [https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1996.0030]
  • Van Dyke, J. and B. McElree. 2006. Retrieval interference in sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language 55(2), 157-166. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2006.03.007]
  • Van Dyke, J. and B. McElree. 2011. Cue-dependent interference in comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language 65, 247-263. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2011.05.002]
  • Van Valin, R. D. Jr. 1995. Toward a functionalist account of so-called extraction constraints. In B. Devriendt, L. Goossens and J. van der Auwera, eds., Complex Structures: A Functionalist Perspective 26-60. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. [https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110815894.29]
  • Villata, S. and J. Franck. 2020. Similarity-based interference in agreement comprehension and production: Evidence from object agreement. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 46(1), 170-188. [https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000718]
  • Warren, T. and E. Gibson. 1999. The Effects of Discourse Status on Intuitive Complexity: Implications for Quantifying Distance in a Locality-based Theory of Linguistic Complexity. In The 12th CUNY Human Sentence Processing, New York, NY.
  • Warren, T. and E. Gibson. 2002. The influence of referential processing on sentence complexity. Cognition 85, 79-112. [https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00087-2]
  • Warren, T. and E. Gibson. 2005. Effects of NP type in reading cleft sentences in English. Language and Cognitive Processes 20(6), 751-767. [https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960500051055]
  • Zehr, J. and F. Schwarz. 2018. PennController for internet-based experiments (IBEX).