The Korean Association for the Study of English Language and Linguistics
[ Article ]
Korea Journal of English Language and Linguistics - Vol. 24, No. 0, pp.305-324
ISSN: 1598-1398 (Print) 2586-7474 (Online)
Print publication date 31 Jan 2024
Received 06 Dec 2023 Revised 20 Dec 2023 Accepted 11 Apr 2024
DOI: https://doi.org/10.15738/kjell.24..202404.305

Cultural and Linguistic Sensitivity in Second Language Development: A Case of Korean L1 Prepubescent Learners of English

Eunjung Kim ; Mun-Hong Choe
(1st author) English Teacher, Gwangju Mujin Middle School kej12020@gmail.com
(corresponding author) Professor, Dept. of English Language Education, Chonnam National University munhong@jnu.ac.kr


© 2024 KASELL All rights reserved
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons License, which permits unrestricted non-commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Abstract

This case study has observed three Korean-speaking prepubescent learners of English as a second language (L2) in the United States over a year, with the goal of investigating the relationship between their ability to notice and accommodate cultural differences and their development in L2 proficiency. Using a mixed method combining quantitative scales, interviews, and observation notes, the study seeks to identify the role that age of immersion plays in the learners’ adaptation to a second culture and the extent to which individual differences in cultural and linguistic sensitivity impact their L2 learning. Results from the measurement scales support that individual differences in cultural and linguistic sensitivity predict L2 development to a significant degree. In particular, those who have a higher transcultural sensitivity in affective and behavioral dimensions are likely to achieve more in L2 proficiency. Also, those who show a preference for the productive-creative and interpersonal-practical functions of language develop faster in oral communication skills than those who are oriented toward ideational-intellectual functions. Qualitative observations suggest that the characteristic differences in the young learners’ second culture and language learning can be described, albeit rather impressionistically, in terms of a gradual change from immersive to observational learning and from skill learning through nonreflective rehearsing to knowledge building through meaning-based representing.

Keywords:

cultural sensitivity, linguistic sensitivity, second language acquisition, individual difference factor, age of onset

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the Ministry of Education of the Republic of Korea and the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF-2022S1A5A2A01038147).

References

  • Andreou, E., G. Andreou and F. Vlachos. 2008. Learning styles and performance in second language tasks. TESOL Quarterly 42(4), 665-674. [https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1545-7249.2008.tb00156.x]
  • Ang, S., T. Rockstuhl and M. L. Tan. 2015. Cultural intelligence and competencies. International Encyclopedia of Social and Behavioral Sciences 2, 433-439. [https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.25050-2]
  • Barjesteh, H. and R. Vaseghi. 2012. Acculturation model for L2 acquisition: Review and evaluation. Advances in Asian Social Science 2(4), 579-584.
  • Bennett, M. J. 1993. Towards ethnorelativism: A developmental model of intercultural sensitivity. In P. M. Paige, ed., Education for the Intercultural Experience, 21-71. Yarmouth, ME: Intercultural Press.
  • Bhawuk, D. P. and R. Brislin. 1992. The measurement of intercultural sensitivity using the concepts of individualism and collectivism. International Journal of Intercultural Relations 16(4), 413-436. [https://doi.org/10.1016/0147-1767(92)90031-O]
  • Bokander, L. and E. Bylund. 2020. Probing the internal validity of the LLAMA language aptitude tests. Language Learning 70(1), 11-47. [https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12368]
  • Bourdieu, P. 1991. Language and Symbolic Power. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
  • Brown, H. D. 1980. The optimal distance model of second language acquisition. TESOL Quarterly 14(2), 157-164. [https://doi.org/10.2307/3586310]
  • Byram, M. 2020. Teaching and Assessing Intercultural Communicative Competence: Revisited. Clevedon, England: Multilingual Matters. [https://doi.org/10.21832/BYRAM0244]
  • Carroll, J. B. and S. M. Sapon. 1959. Modern Language Aptitude Test (MLAT). New York: The Psychological Corporation.
  • Castells, M. 2009. Communication Power. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Castro, O. and V. Peck. 2005. Learning styles and foreign language learning difficulties. Foreign Language Annals 38(3), 401-409. [https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1944-9720.2005.tb02226.x]
  • Choe, M.-H. 2022a. Second culture acquisition and language development: Cultural sensitivity as an individual difference factor. The Linguistic Association of Korea Journal 30(4), 145-167.
  • Choe, M.-H. 2022b. Profiling second language learners’ linguistic sensitivity through a self-reporting questionnaire. Lingua Humanitatis 24(2), 469-497. [https://doi.org/10.16945/inahsl.24.2.18]
  • Choe, M.-H. 2023. Sensitivity in cultural differences and second language acquisition: The case of Uzbek students of L2 Korean. Studies in Foreign Language Education 37(1), 179-196.
  • Deardorff, D. 2011. Assessing intercultural competence. New Directions for Institutional Research 2011(149), 65-79. [https://doi.org/10.1002/ir.381]
  • DeKeyser, R., I. Alfi-Shabtay and D. Ravid. 2010. Cross-linguistic evidence for the nature of age effects in second language acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics 31, 413-438. [https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716410000056]
  • Dörnyei, Z. 2010. Researching motivation: From integrativeness to the ideal L2 self. Introducing Applied Linguistics: Concepts and Skills 3(5), 74-83.
  • Dörnyei, Z. and E. Ushioda. 2009. Motivation, Language Identity and the L2 Self. Clevedon, England: Multilingual Matters. [https://doi.org/10.21832/9781847691293]
  • Doughty, C. J. 2014. Assessing aptitude. In A. Kunnan, ed., The Companion to Language Assessment, 25-47. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.
  • Doughty, C. J. 2019. Cognitive language aptitude. Language Learning 69(1), 101-126. [https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12322]
  • Ehrman, M. E., B. L. Leaver and R. L. Oxford. 2003. A brief overview of individual differences in second language learning. System 31(3), 313-330. [https://doi.org/10.1016/S0346-251X(03)00045-9]
  • Granena, G. and M. H. Long. 2013. Sensitive Periods, Language Aptitude, and Ultimate L2 Attainment. Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing. [https://doi.org/10.1075/lllt.35]
  • Hammer, M. R., M. J. Bennett and R. Wiseman. 2003. Measuring intercultural sensitivity: The intercultural development inventory. International Journal of Intercultural Relations 27(4), 421-443. [https://doi.org/10.1016/S0147-1767(03)00032-4]
  • Hatami, S. 2013. Learning styles. ELT Journal 67(4), 488-490. [https://doi.org/10.1093/elt/ccs083]
  • Kordes, H. 1991. Intercultural learning at school: Limits and possibilities. In D. Buttjes and M. Byram, eds., Mediating Languages and Cultures, 17-30. Clevedon, England: Multilingual Matters.
  • Kramsch, C. 2014. Language and culture. AILA Review 27(1), 30-55. [https://doi.org/10.1075/aila.27.02kra]
  • Lantolf, J. P. 1999. Second culture acquisition: Cognitive considerations. In E. Hinkel, ed., Culture in Second Language Teaching and Learning, 28-46. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Lantolf, J. P. 2000. Sociocultural Theory and Second Language Learning (Vol. 78, No. 4). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Lantolf, J. P. and T. G. Beckett. 2009. Sociocultural theory and second language acquisition. Language Teaching 42(4), 459-475. [https://doi.org/10.1017/S0261444809990048]
  • Li, S. 2016. The construct valididty of language aptitude: A meta-analysis. Studies in Second Langauge Acquisition 38(4), 801-842. [https://doi.org/10.1017/S027226311500042X]
  • Linck, J. A., M. M. Hughes, S. G. Campbell, N. H. Silbert, M. Tare, S. R. Jackson and C. J. Doughty. 2013. Hi-LAB: A new measure of aptitude for high-level language proficiency. Language Learning 63(3), 530–566. [https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12011]
  • Makransky, G. and G. B. Petersen. 2021. The cognitive affective model of immersive learning (CAMIL): A theoretical research-based model of learning in immersive virtual reality. Educational Psychology Review 33, 1-22. [https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-020-09586-2]
  • Meara, P. 2005. LLAMA Language Aptitude Tests. Swansea, UK: Lognostics. [https://doi.org/10.1037/t88467-000]
  • Medina, J. 2003. Identity trouble: Disidentification and the problem of difference. Philosophy and Social Criticism 29(6), 655-680. [https://doi.org/10.1177/0191453703296002]
  • Minoura, Y. 1992. A sensitive period for the incorporation of a cultural meaning system: A study of Japanese children growing up in the United States. Ethos 20(3), 304-339. [https://doi.org/10.1525/eth.1992.20.3.02a00030]
  • Moyer, A. 1999. Ultimate attainment in L2 phonology: The critical factors of age, motivation, and instruction. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 21(1), 81-108. [https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263199001035]
  • Ott, D. L. and S. Michailova. 2018. Cultural intelligence: A review and new research avenues. International Journal of Management Reviews 20(1), 99-119. [https://doi.org/10.1111/ijmr.12118]
  • Oxford, R. L. 1990. Language Learning Strategies: What Every Teacher Should Know. New York: Newbury House/Harper & Row.
  • Paulhus, D. L. and S. Vazire. 2007. The self-report method. In R.W. Robins, R. C. Fraley and R. F. Krueger, eds., Handbook of Research Methods in Personality Psychology, 224-239. New York: Guilford Press
  • Robinson, G. L. 1991. Second culture acquisition. In J. E. Alatis, ed., Georgetown University Round Table on Language and Linguistics, 114-122. Washington DC: Georgetown University.
  • Robinson, P. 2013. Individual differences, aptitude complexes, SLA processes, and aptitude test development. In M. Pawlak, ed., New Perspectives on Individual Differences in Language Learning and Teaching, 57-75. Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag. [https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-20850-8_4]
  • Robinson, P. 2019. Aptitude and second language acquisition. In C. Chapelle, ed., The Concise Encyclopedia of Applied Linguistics, 129-133. Oxford: Blackwell. [https://doi.org/10.1002/9781405198431.wbeal0035.pub2]
  • Robinson‐Stuart, G. L. and H. Nocon. 1996. Second culture acquisition: Ethnography in the foreign language classroom. The Modern Language Journal 80(4), 431-449. [https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4781.1996.tb05463.x]
  • Schumann, J. H. 1978. The Pidginization Process: A Model for Second Language Acquisition. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
  • Schumann, J. H. 1990. Extending the scope of the acculturation/pidginization model to include cognition. TESOL Quarterly 24(4), 667-684. [https://doi.org/10.2307/3587114]
  • Skehan, P. 2012. Language aptitude. In S. Gass and A. Mackey, eds., Routledge Handbook of Second Language Acquisition, 381-395. New York: Routledge.
  • Skehan, P. 2016. Foreign language aptitude, acquisitional sequences, and psycholinguistic processes. In G. Granena, D. Jackson and Y. Yilmaz, eds., Cognitive Individual Differences in L2 Processing and Acquisition, 17-40. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. [https://doi.org/10.1075/bpa.3.02ske]
  • Suinn, R. M., G. Khoo and C. Ahuna. 1995. The Suinn‐Lew Asian self‐identity acculturation scale: Cross‐cultural information. Journal of Multicultural Counseling and Development 23(3), 139-148. [https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2161-1912.1995.tb00269.x]
  • Talmy, L. 2000. Toward a Cognitive Semantics: Typology and Process in Concept Structuring. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/6848.001.0001]
  • Tummala-Narra, P. 2015. Cultural competence as a core emphasis of psychoanalytic psychotherapy. Psychoanalytic Psychology 32(2), 275-292. [https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034041]
  • Willing, K. 1988. Learning Styles in Adult Migrant Education. Adelaide, South Australia: NCRC.
  • Wong, L. L. and D. Nunan. 2011. The learning styles and strategies of effective language learners. System 39(2), 144-163. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2011.05.004]