The Korean Association for the Study of English Language and Linguistics
[ Article ]
Korea Journal of English Language and Linguistics - Vol. 24, No. 0, pp.483-495
ISSN: 1598-1398 (Print) 2586-7474 (Online)
Print publication date 31 Jan 2024
Received 28 Nov 2023 Revised 27 Dec 2023 Accepted 13 May 2024
DOI: https://doi.org/10.15738/kjell.24..202404.483

A Masked Priming Study on Morphological Parsing in Lexical Access by Korean Learners of L2 English

Jeonghwa Shin
Associate Professor Department of English Korea Military Academy 574 Hwarang-Ro, Nowon-gu Seoul 01805, Korea jshinling@gmail.com


© 2024 KASELL All rights reserved
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons License, which permits unrestricted non-commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Abstract

This study examines how word composition and phonological transparency affect the processing of L2 lexical items. To investigate this, Korean-speaking learners of English were engaged in a lexical decision task using a forward masked priming paradigm. The task involved three types of words: regular inflections, phonologically transparent derivations, and phonologically opaque derivations (e.g., boil, blind, brutal). During the experiment, participants were presented with a forward mask (######), followed by a prime word (e.g., boiled), and finally a target word (e.g., BOIL). Participants had to make lexical decisions for the target word. The prime word was identical to the target word (e.g., boil-BOIL), a suffixed form (e.g., boiled-BOIL), or unrelated (e.g., troll-BOIL). The results showed no significant differences in accuracy across the different word types. However, the response latency was longest for phonologically opaque derivations, followed by regular inflections, and shortest for phonologically transparent derivations. Additionally, the unrelated prime resulted in the longest response latency, while the suffixed prime led to longer reaction times than the identical prime. This priming effect was only observed for derivations, particularly phonologically transparent ones. These findings suggest that the processing of English derivational words by Korean L2 English speakers involves morphological decomposition, with a higher parsing cost for phonologically opaque derivations compared to phonologically transparent ones. In contrast, inflected forms did not show evidence of structural parsing in the L2 learners, indicating that regular inflections are stored as whole words by Korean learners of English.

Keywords:

forward masked priming, lexical decision, morphological decomposition, L2 lexical access

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by 2023 research fund of Korea Military Academy (Hwarangdae Research Institute) (RN: 2023B1020). I also would like to appreciate the useful comments that three reviewers provided for this paper. Any remaining errors are my own.

References

  • Andrews, S., B. Miller and K. Rayner. 2004. Eye movements and morphological segmentation of compound words: There is a mouse in mousetrap. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 16(1-2), 285-311. [https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440340000123]
  • Balota, D. A., M. J. Yap, K. A. Hutchison, M. J. Cortese, B. Kessler, B. Loftis and R. Treiman. 2007. The English lexicon project. Behavior Research Methods 39, 445-459. [https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193014]
  • Cheesman, J. and P. M. Merikle. 1986. Distinguishing conscious from unconscious perceptual processes. Canadian Journal of Psychology 40(4), 343. [https://doi.org/10.1037/h0080103]
  • Clahsen, H. 1999. Lexical entries and rules of language: A multidisciplinary study of German inflection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22, 991-1060. [https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X99002228]
  • Clahsen, H. 2006. Linguistic perspectives on morphological processing. In D. Wunderlich, ed., Advances in the Theory of the Lexicon, 355-388. Mouton de Gruyter: Berlin. [https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110197815.355]
  • Clahsen, H. and K. Neubauer. 2010. Morphology, frequency, and the processing of derived words in native and non-native speakers. Lingua 120(11), 2627-2637. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2010.06.007]
  • Coughlin, C. E. and A. Tremblay. 2015. Morphological decomposition in native and non-native French speakers. Bilingualism: Language & Cognition 18, 524-542. [https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728914000200]
  • Dal Maso, S. and H. Giraudo. 2014. Morphological processing in L2 Italian: Evidence from a masked priming study. Lingvisticae Investigationes 37, 322-337. [https://doi.org/10.1075/li.37.2.09mas]
  • Debner, J. A. and L. L. Jacoby. 1994. Unconscious perception: Attention, awareness, and control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 20(2), 304-317. [https://doi.org/10.1037//0278-7393.20.2.304]
  • Feldman, L. B., A. Kostić, D. M. Basnight-Brown, D. Filipović Đurđević and M. J. Pastizzo. 2010. Morphological facilitation for regular and irregular verb formations in native and nonnative speakers: Little evidence for two distinct mechanisms. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 13, 119-135. [https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728909990459]
  • Fiorentino, R. and D. Poeppel. 2007. Processing of compound words: An MEG study. Brain and Language 1(103), 18-19. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2007.07.009]
  • Forster, K. I. and C. Davis. 1984. Repetition priming and frequency attenuation in lexical access. Journal of experimental psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 10(4), 680-698. [https://doi.org/10.1037//0278-7393.10.4.680]
  • Forster, K. I., C. Davis, C. Schoknecht and R. Carter. 1987. Masked priming with graphemically related forms: Repetition or partial activation? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 39(2), 211-251. [https://doi.org/10.1080/14640748708401785]
  • Jackendoff, R. and S. Pinker. 2005. The nature of the language faculty and its implications for evolution of language. Cognition 97, 211-225. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005.04.006]
  • Marcel, A. J. 1983. Conscious and unconscious perception: Experiments on visual masking and word recognition. Cognitive psychology 15(2), 197-237. [https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(83)90009-9]
  • McClelland, J. L. and K. Patterson. 2002. Rules or connections in past-tense inflections: What does the evidence rule out? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6, 465-472. [https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01993-9]
  • Neubauer, K. and H. Clahsen. 2009. Decomposition of inflected words in a second language: An experimental study of German participles. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 31(3), 403-435. [https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263109090354]
  • Penke, M. 2006. The representation of inflectional morphology in the mental lexicon. An overview of psycho-and neurolinguistic methods and results. Advances in the Theory of the Lexicon, 389-428. [https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110197815.389]
  • Pinker, S. 1999. Words and Rules: The Ingredients of Language. New York: Basic Books.
  • Pinker, S. and M. T. Ullman. 2002. The past and future of the past tense. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6, 456- 463. [https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01990-3]
  • Pollatsek, A. and J. Hyönä. 2005. The role of semantic transparency in the processing of Finnish compound words. Language and Cognitive processes 20(1-2), 261-290. [https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960444000098]
  • Prasada, S. and S. Pinker. 1993. Generalisation of regular and irregular morphological patterns. Language and Cognitive Processes 8, 1-56. [https://doi.org/10.1080/01690969308406948]
  • Prasada, S., S. Pinker and S. William. 1990. Some evidence that irregular forms are retrieved from memory but regular forms are rule-generated. Paper presented at the 31st Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society.
  • Psychology Software Tools, Inc. 2016. E-Prime (version 3.0) [computer software]. Available online at https://support.pstnet.com/
  • Seidenberg, M. S. and L. M. Gonnerman. 2000. Explaining derivational morphology as the convergence of codes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4, 353-361. [https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01515-1]
  • Silva, R. and H. Clahsen. 2008. Morphologically complex words in L1 and L2 processing: Evidence from masked priming experiments in English. Bilingualism: Language and cognition 11(2), 245-260. [https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728908003404]
  • Studebaker, G. A. 1985. A" rationalized" arcsine transform. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 28(3), 455-462. [https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.2803.455]
  • Taft, M. 2004. Morphological decomposition and the reverse base frequency effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A 57(4), 745-765. [https://doi.org/10.1080/02724980343000477]
  • Voga, M., A. Anastassiadis-Symeonidis and H. Giraudo. 2014. Does morphology play a role in L2 processing?: Two masked priming experiments with Greek speakers of ESL. Lingvisticae Investigationes 37(2), 338-352. [https://doi.org/10.1075/li.37.2.10vog]