The Korean Association for the Study of English Language and Linguistics
[ Article ]
Korea Journal of English Language and Linguistics - Vol. 24, No. 0, pp.754-771
ISSN: 1598-1398 (Print) 2586-7474 (Online)
Print publication date 31 Jan 2024
Received 04 Jul 2024 Revised 17 Jul 2024 Accepted 22 Jul 2024
DOI: https://doi.org/10.15738/kjell.24..202407.754

Individual Differences as Predictors of L2 writing: L2 Proficiency, Working Memory Capacity, and Explicit Language Aptitude

Jiyong Lee
Assistant Professor, Department of English Korea Military Academy KMA, Chungmukwan #472, Hwarang-Ro 574, Nowon-Gu Seoul, Korea, Tel: 02) 2197-2637 jlee0123@kma.ac.kr


© 2024 KASELL All rights reserved
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons License, which permits unrestricted non-commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Abstract

Much attention has been paid to the effects of individual differences on L2 learning in recent years, but there is still a lack of studies that directly investigated the roles that more cognitive individual differences play in L2 writing. The present study investigated whether L2 proficiency, working memory capacity (WMC), and explicit language aptitude (ELA) could predict L2 writing, in terms of syntactic complexity, lexical diversity, and accuracy. Because L2 writing requires conscious cognitive effort, the study also sought to find out whether these individual differences could also predict the cognitive load imposed on the learner during L2 writing. Forty-two Korean EFL learners completed a series of an English proficiency test, a writing task, a questionnaire measuring cognitive load, a test of ELA, and a test of WMC. Results revealed that those with greater L2 proficiency and explicit language aptitude perceived the writing task as more difficult and/or stressful. Regarding written performance, higher L2 proficiency was linked to greater accuracy, and higher WMC was associated with greater lexical diversity. These results have implications for our understanding of the cognitive processes and demands of L2 writing.

Keywords:

L2 writing, L2 proficiency, working memory capacity, explicit language aptitude

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the Ministry of Education of the Republic of Korea and the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF-2021S1A5B5A16076199).

References

  • Abu-Rabia, S. 2003. The influence of working memory on reading and creative writing processes in a second language. Educational Psychology 23(2), 209-222. [https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410303227]
  • Ali, S. M. and K. S. Hussein. 2014. The comparative power of type/token and hapax legomena/type tatios: A corpus-based study of authorial differentiation. Advances in Language and Literary Studies 5(3), 112-119. [https://doi.org/10.7575/aiac.alls.v.5n.3p.112]
  • Atkins, S. M. 2011. Working memory assessment and training (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Maryland, College Park, MD.
  • Baddeley, A. 2000. The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4(11), 417-423. [https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01538-2]
  • Baddeley, A. 2003. Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 4(10), 829-839. [https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1201]
  • Baddeley, A., and G. Hitch. 1974. Working memory. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation 8, 47-89. [https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60452-1]
  • Benzehaf, B. 2017. Comparing learners’ general proficiency levels with their writing productive ability: How correlated are they? Eurasian Journal of Applied Linguistics 3(2), 43-58. [https://doi.org/10.32601/ejal.460961]
  • Bokander, L., and E. Bylund. 2020. Probing the internal validity of the LLAMA language aptitude tests. Language Learning 70(1), 11-47. [https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12368]
  • Brown, J. D. 1980. Relative merits of four methods for scoring cloze tests. The Modern Language Journal 64(3), 311-317. [https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4781.1980.tb05198.x]
  • Carroll, J. B. 1989. The Carroll model a 25-year retrospective and prospective view. Educational Researcher 18(1), 26-31. [https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X018001026]
  • Carroll, J. B. 1990. Cognitive abilities in foreign language aptitude: Then and now. In T. S. Parry and C. W. Stansfield, eds., Language Aptitude Reconsidered, 11-9. Washington, DC: Center for Applied Linguistics.
  • Chrabaszcz, A. and N. Jiang. 2014. The role of the native language in the use of the English nongeneric definite article by L2 learners: A cross-linguistic comparison. Second Language Research 30(3), 351-379. [https://doi.org/10.1177/0267658313493432]
  • Conway, A. R., M. J. Kane, M. F. Bunting, D. Z. Hambrick, O. Wilhelm and R.W. Engle. 2005. Working memory span tasks: A methodological review and user’s guide. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 12(5), 769-786. [https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196772]
  • Crosthwaite, P. 2016. L2 English article use by L1 speakers of article-less languages: A learner corpus study. International Journal of Learner Corpus Research 2(1), 68-100. [https://doi.org/10.1075/ijlcr.2.1.03cro]
  • DeKeyser, R. M. 2005. What makes learning second‐language grammar difficult? A review of issues. Language Learning 55(S1), 1-25. [https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0023-8333.2005.00294.x]
  • DeKeyser, R. M. 2007. Skill acquisition theory. In B. VanPatten and J. Williams, eds., Theories in Second Language Acquisition: An Introduction, 97-113. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  • DeKeyser, R. M. and J. Koeth. 2011. Cognitive aptitudes for second language learning. Handbook of Research in Second Language Teaching and Learning 2, 395-406.
  • Dixon, C. N. and D. D. Nessel. 1983. Language Experience Approach to Reading and Writing: Language Experience Reading for Second Language Learners. Hayward, CA: Alemany Press.
  • Engle, R. W., M. J. Kane and S. W. Tuholski. 1999. Individual differences in working memory capacity and what they tell us about controlled attention, general fluid intelligence and functions of the prefrontal cortex. In A. Miyake and P. Shah, eds., Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control, 102-132. Cambridge University Press. [https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139174909.007]
  • Flower, L. S. and J. R. Hayes. 1980. The dynamics of composing: Making plans and juggling constraints. In L. W. Gregg and E. R. Steinberg, eds., Cognitive Processes in Writing: An Interdisciplinary Approach, 31-50. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum.
  • Galbraith, D. and I. Vedder. 2019. Methodological advances in investigating L2 writing processes: Challenges and perspectives. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 41(3), 633-645. [https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263119000366]
  • Granena, G. 2013. Cognitive aptitudes for second language learning and the LLAMA Language Aptitude Test. In G. Granena and M. H. Long, eds., Sensitive Periods, Language Aptitude, and Ultimate L2 Attainment, 105-129. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. [https://doi.org/10.1075/lllt.35]
  • Guiraud, P. 1954. Les Charactères Statistiques du Vocabulaire. Essai de Méthodologie. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.
  • Hayes, J. R. and L. S. Flower. 1980. Identifying the Organization of Writing Process. In L. W. Gregg and E. R. Steinberg, eds., Cognitive Process in Writing, 3-30. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  • Hunt, K. 1964. Differences in Grammatical Structures Written at Three Grade Levels (Cooperative Research Project No. 1998). Tallahassee: Florida State University.
  • Kellogg, R. T. 1996. A model of working memory in writing. In C. M. Levy and S. Ransdell, eds., The Science of Writing: Theories, Methods, Individual Differences, and Applications, 57-71. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
  • Kellogg, R. T. 1999. The Psychology of Writing. Oxford University Press. [https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780195129083.001.0001]
  • Kormos, J. 2023. The role of cognitive factors in second language writing and writing to learn a second language. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 45(3), 622-646. [https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263122000481]
  • Kormos, J. 2012. The role of individual differences in L2 writing. Journal of Second Language Writing 21(4), 390-403. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jslw.2012.09.003]
  • Kormos, J. and A. Sáfár. 2008. Phonological short-term memory, working memory and foreign language performance in intensive language learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 11(2), 261-271. [https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728908003416]
  • Kormos, J. and A. Trebits. 2012. The role of task complexity, modality, and aptitude in narrative task performance. Language Learning 62(2), 439-472. [https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2012.00695.x]
  • Kuiken, F. and I. Vedder. 2007. Task complexity and measures of linguistic performance in L2 writing. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching 45(3), 261-284. [https://doi.org/10.1515/iral.2007.012]
  • Kuiken, F. and I. Vedder. 2008. Cognitive task complexity and written output in Italian and French as a foreign language. Journal of Second Language Writing 17(1), 48-60. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jslw.2007.08.003]
  • Kuiken, F. and I. Vedder. 2011. Task complexity and linguistic performance in L2 writing and speaking. In P. Robinson, ed., Second Language Task Complexity: Researching the Cognition Hypothesis of Language Learning and Performance, 91-104. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company. [https://doi.org/10.1075/tblt.2.09ch4]
  • Kurilecz, M. 1969. Man and His World: A Structured Reader. New York: Crowell.
  • Lardilleux, A. and Y. Lepage. 2007. Hapax Legomena: Their contribution in number and efficiency to word alignment. In Z. Vetulani and H. Uszkoreit, eds., Human Language Technology: Challenges of the Information Society. Language and Technology Conference, 440-450. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. [https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-04235-5_38]
  • Lee, J. 2019. Task Complexity, cognitive load, and L1 speech, Applied Linguistics 40(3), 506-539. [https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/amx054]
  • Manchón, R. M., S. McBride, M. D. M. Martínez and O. Vasylets. 2023. Working memory, L2 proficiency, and task complexity: Independent and interactive effects on L2 written performance. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 45(3), 737-764. [https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263123000141]
  • Meara, P. 2005. LLAMA language aptitude tests: The manual. Swansea: Lognostics. [https://doi.org/10.1037/t88467-000]
  • Mikawa, M. and N. H. De Jong. 2021. Language neutrality of the LLAMA test explored: The case of agglutinative languages and multiple writing systems. Journal of the European Second Language Association 5(1), 87-100. [https://doi.org/10.22599/jesla.71]
  • Nair, V. and S. Sircar. 2021. Task complexity and language proficiency: Its effect on L2 writing production. In N. P. Sudharshana and L. Mukhopadhyay, eds., Task-Based Language Teaching and Assessment, 139-161. Springer, Singapore. [https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-16-4226-5_8]
  • Nawal, A. F. 2018. Cognitive load theory in the context of second language academic writing. Higher Education Pedagogies 3(1), 385-402. [https://doi.org/10.1080/23752696.2018.1513812]
  • Ortega, L. 2012. Epilogue: Exploring L2 writing–SLA interfaces. Journal of Second Language Writing 21(4), 404-415. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jslw.2012.09.002]
  • Paas, F., J. E. Tuovinen, H. Tabbers and P. W. Van Gerven. 2003. Cognitive load measurement as a means to advance cognitive load theory. Educational psychologist 38(1), 63-71. [https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326985EP3801_8]
  • Pica, T. 1983. The article in American English: What the textbooks don't tell us. In N. Wolfson and E. Judd, eds., Sociolinguistics and Language Acquisition, 222-233. Rowley, MA: Newbury Hou.
  • Robinson, P. 2005. Aptitude and second language acquisition. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics 25, 46-73. [https://doi.org/10.1017/S0267190505000036]
  • Rogers, V., P. Meara, T. Barnett-Legh, C. Curry and E. Davie. 2017. Examining the LLAMA aptitude tests. Journal of the European Second Language Association 1(1), 49-60. [https://doi.org/10.22599/jesla.24]
  • Skehan, P. 2016. Tasks versus conditions: Two perspectives on task research and their implications for pedagogy. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics 36, 34-49. [https://doi.org/10.1017/S0267190515000100]
  • Suzuki, Y. and R. DeKeyser. 2017. The interface of explicit and implicit knowledge in a second language: Insights from individual differences in cognitive aptitudes. Language Learning 67(4), 747-790. [https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12241]
  • Sweller, J., P. Ayres and S. Kalyuga. 2011. Cognitive Load Theory. Berlin: Springer [https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-8126-4]
  • Vasylets, O. and J. Marín. 2021. The effects of working memory and L2 proficiency on L2 writing. Journal of Second Language Writing 52, 100786. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jslw.2020.100786]
  • Yilmaz, Y. and G. Granena. 2016. The role of cognitive aptitudes for explicit language learning in the relative effects of explicit and implicit feedback. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 19(01), 147-161. [https://doi.org/10.1017/S136672891400090X]
  • Zabihi, R. 2018. The role of cognitive and affective factors in measures of L2 writing. Written Communication 35(1), 32-57. [https://doi.org/10.1177/0741088317735836]
  • Zalbidea, J. 2017. ‘One task fits all’? The roles of task complexity, modality, and working memory capacity in L2 performance. The Modern Language Journal 101(2), 335-352. [https://doi.org/10.1111/modl.12389]